A relatively obscure but fascinating study titled, “Does Professor
Quality Matter? Evidence from Random Assignment of Students to
Professors” appeared in the Journal of Political Economy,
2010, vol 118 no 3, also available online
HERE.
From the paper’s abstract:
In primary and secondary education, measures of teacher quality are
often based on contemporaneous student performance on standardized
achievement tests. In the postsecondary environment, scores on
student evaluations of professors are typically used to measure
teaching quality. We possess unique data that allow us to measure
relative student performance in mandatory follow-on classes. We
compare metrics that capture these three different notions of
instructional quality and present evidence that professors
who excel at promoting contemporaneous student achievement teach in
ways that improve their student evaluations but harm the follow-on
achievement of their students in more advanced classes.
(Bolding, ours.)
In the report,
In contrast, the students of more experienced and more highly
qualified introductory professors perform significantly better in
the follow-on courses.
The authors go even further with respect to the approach of
professors who are seen to improve the follow-on achievement of
their students:
One potential explanation for our results is that the less
experienced professors may adhere more strictly to the regimented
curriculum being tested, whereas the more experienced professors
broaden the curriculum and produce students with a deeper
understanding of the material. This deeper understanding results in
better achievement in the follow-on courses.
Unfortunately,
Student evaluations are positively correlated with contemporaneous
professor value-added and negatively correlated with follow-on
student achievement. That is, students appear to reward higher
grades in the introductory course but punish professors who increase
deep learning.
What are we to make of this? Many things, probably. One obvious
conclusion is that positive student evaluations do not correlate to
long-term student success. As Stanly Fish writes,
Students tend to like everything neatly laid out; they want
to know exactly where they are; they don’t welcome the introduction
of multiple perspectives, especially when no master perspective
reconciles them; they want the answers. But sometimes (although not
always) effective teaching involves the deliberate inducing of
confusion, the withholding of clarity, the refusal to provide
answers; sometimes a class or an entire semester is spent being
taken down various garden paths leading to dead ends that require
inquiry to begin all over again, with the same discombobulating
result; sometimes your expectations have been systematically
disappointed. … (from
Deep in the Heart of Texas,
The New York Times, June 21, 2010 )
Seen from the other direction, then, negative student evaluations do
not necessarily tell us that we’re not contributing significantly to
long-term student success. Setting aside concern about how student
evaluations are used by institutions, how do WE use them? What do we
glean from them? If there is negative correlation between
evaluations and long-term student success, can we rely on that
relationship? Do we ignore them as a necessary nuisance? Or do we
take from them what we can, while striving to not stray into the
‘positive evaluation = short-term success’ category?
What IS a Process Educator to make of student evaluations in
light of this study?
|